DEVELOPMENT & MEMORY (BOOK 4)

Brief notes listing the main features - more details in relevant sections (§) of Book 4.

Return to Tutorial FIVE notes
 

NEURONAL DEVELOPMENT (Chs 2 & 3)

1. Proliferation Cell division - initial increase in cell numbers in embryo. (§2.2)

2. Differentiation and 3. Migration of neurons (§2.3)

Unspecialised embryonic neurons (neuroblasts) must differentiate into neurons and also migrate to their 'final' position. These events occur at roughly the same time. Differentiation involves switching on particular genes (induction), which can then be expressed.

How a given cell differentiates depends on its:

(a) lineage - (its 'ancestry'). Time of development is also important; e.g. in rat forebrain, the later developing cells migrate furthest - to the outer cortex.

(b) local environment - the particular signals that the cells receive during development affect their subsequent development; e.g. if early cortical cells are transplanted to a different part of the brain, they develop according to this new position, rather than where they came from originally.

4. Axon growth (§2.4)

Neurons make connections via their axon processes. Axons must first grow and then establish functional connections (synapses) with appropriate targets. Neuroblasts develop several small processes (neurites), one of which becomes the axon (the rest become dendrites). Axonal growth can occur once a neuron reaches its final position or during migration.

Growth cones (the growing tip of an axon) must be stimulated to grow, and also to grow in the right direction towards the 'target'. Several factors are involved:-

Chemotactic factors act locally - helping the axon to 'feel its way'; chemotropic factors act at a distance ["tropic" = "growing towards"], and the neuron homes in on this 'beacon'. Distinguish from chemotrophic (survival) factors ["trophic" = "feeding"] - see below.
Chemotactic factors: Cell adhesion molecules and receptors; laminin.
Glial cells also play a role (by supplying chemotrophic factors)
Chemotropic factors: Guidepost cells (§ 2.4.4); "Max factor" (§ 2.4.5)

The timing is important: growth factors are not produced continuously, and neurons can only respond at certain periods. Some factors appear to inhibit growth.
Axon guidance is a complex process: axons are affected by their environment and in turn can affect their environment.

5. Synaptogenesis (Ch 3)

Formation of functional contacts (synapses) with target cells (e.g. other neurons, muscles, etc.) Not all neurons survive. Survival is selective and involves 'matching' neurons with their targets.

Even amongst neurons that reach their targets, there is massive cell death. Neurons 'die' if they fail to reach a target, or if they fail to maintain functional contact.(§3.2.1)
To survive, they need:
functional synapses: neural activity is necessary for survival (§ 3.1);
chemotrophic (survival) factors (e.g. Nerve Growth Factor) (§ 3.2.2);
appropriate targets: neurons that reach the 'wrong' target die (§ 3.2.3, 3.2.4). Again, neural activity (action potentials) is important in the selection process.

6. Refinement of synapses (§ 3.3, 3.4)
Selective pruning of inappropriate connections and strengthening of functional synapses.

a. Afterent (peripheral) inputs are important (e.g. whisker barrels §3.3.1: muscle afferents §3.3.2)
b. Selective reduction ('pruning') of axonal branches making inappropriate projections, e.g. projections from areas of the cerebral cortex to pyramidal tracts in rats, and refining connections between right and left visual cortices through the corpus callosum (§3.4.1)
c. Functional tuning to reduce the extent of terminal branches within target area: e.g. retinal ganglion cell projection to LGN (§ 3.4.2).

Neural activity is necessary for this final process, perhaps by selectively strengthening some synapses at the expense of others (§ 3.4.3) (cf. memory formation - see later in course)
 

growth of retinal ganglion cell axons into LGN
 
 
 

Axonal remodelling in the lateral geniculate nucleus occurs largely before birth. As shown in this figure, at the earliest times in development (1), the axons from the left eye and right eye are simple and tipped with growth cones. The shaded region represents the intermixing of inputs from both eyes. After further development (2), the axons grow many side branches. The axons soon begin to lose some side branches and start to extend elaborate terminal branches (3). Eventually these branches occupy the appropriate territory to form eye-specific layers (4).

See also Fig. 3.9 in Book 4.
 
 
 
 

Back to tutorial 5 notes
 


PLASTICITY: GROWTH & REPAIR

Most neuronal growth occurs during development; to what extent do neurons continue to grow in the mature animal?

There is little evidence that neurons divide in mature mammals; continued growth and replacement of olfactory neurons is an exception.
Regeneration can occur in the peripheral nervous system (PNS) and central nervous system (CNS) in fish and amphibia. (§ 3.5.1, 3.5.3) Neuronal regeneration can occur in mammalian PNS, but not CNS.
The types of glia present are important: Schwann cells (PNS) promote regeneration; CNS glia (oligodendrocytes) inhibit regrowth.
The intact central part of injured peripheral axons can regenerate and grow along the glial (Schwann cell) sheaths of the degenerated peripheral portion. (§3.5.2) Such regrowth does not occur in the mammalian CNS (§3.5.4). BUT see the recent "Stop Press" from the Course Team.

Evidence of plasticity in mature mammalian NS

(Plastic is opposite of elastic; plastic things deform under external forces e.g. 'Plasticene' and keep their new shape, elastic springs back to its original shape.)

expanding receptive fields
Peripheral axonal damage is accompanied by sprouting of adjacent axon collaterals, which can take over the denervated target (§3.6.1). In time, the injured axon may regenerate and reestablish its receptive field. It has to compete with the adjacent collaterals for target space.

From Fig. 3.14 of Book 4.
 
 
 
 

Removing peripheral inputs to dorsal horn cells (by cutting dorsal roots - §3.6.2) results in altered receptive fields of the DH cells - axonal sprouting and/or unmasking of 'silent' synapses already there.
 
 

new receptive fields
Removing inputs to cortical cells by cutting peripheral nerves (e.g. median nerve - §3.6.3). Cortical cells no longer respond to stimulation of original RF. But these neurons soon develop new RFs adjacent to their original ones - due to axonal sprouting or unmasking of 'silent' synapses.
 
 

From Fig. 3.16 of Book 4.
 
 
 

Back to tutorial 5 notes


DEVELOPMENT OF BEHAVIOUR (Ch 4)

Development of the nervous system is likely to influence behaviour.

Factors affecting neural development include: genes, external stimuli and hormones. These in turn can influence subsequent behaviour.

NO ONE FACTOR PREDOMINATES - THEY ALL INTERACT TO VARYING DEGREES.

Genes

Courtship in fruit flies. Experiments with genetic mosaics show that only certain parts of the animal need to have the male genotype for particular 'male' behaviours to be manifest (§4.2.1).
In male sphinx moth, development of the macroglomerular complex requires the present of long antennal sensory organs, which are part of the male genotype (§4.2.2).

External environment - stimuli

General factors (§4.3.1)

Different forms of insect castes can occur in very similar genotypes.
Temporal castes reveal differences in behaviour at different times during development.

Physical differences may arise from:
    Differential (non-isometric) growth
    'Decision points': external factors having particular effects at specific times, e.g. nutrition, environmental temperature, humidity, influence of pheromones.
 

Non-specific stimuli (§4.3.2)

Normally ill-defined events and tend to have complex, inter-related actions.
Adult behaviour (in rats and monkeys) can be influenced during development by:
    enriched/sparse environment;
    extent of handling;
    quality of nutrition;
    social contacts (mother, peers)

Specific stimuli

a. Auditory
Sound localisation is dependent on binaural hearing. Sound localisation in owls is impaired by plugging one ear (monaural plugging)
    up to age 60 days - compensation occurs, and normal localisation is possible;
    after age 60 days - no compensation, but normal localisation restored after 'unplugging'.
b. Visual
Visual experience during early post-natal life can influence organisation of the visual cortex.
Concept of 'sensitive' periods revealed by deprivation experiments at different stages:-

effects of monocular closure
 
 
 
 

Effects of monocular inputs on ocular dominance columns.
Connections within visual cortex adjust so that normally binocular cells become monocular. (see below).

Effects of deprivation can be reversed up to a certain developmental stage (e.g. 3/12 in cats).

Similar effects on orientation columns if animals are reared in special environments devoid of 'vertically' or 'horizontally' oriented 'stimuli'.
 
 
 
 

effect of eye closure

.

SCHEME ILLUSTRATING EFFECTS OF EYE CLOSURE. In the normal cat or monkey by 6 weeks ocular dominance columns have become well defined in layer IV of the cortex. Lid closure causes excessive retraction of fibres supplied by the deprived eye. Those supplied by the open eye retract less than usual and in the adult supply larger areas of cortex than in normal animals where the competition is more equal. After reverse suture during the critical period. the initially deprived eye can recapture the territory it had lost. (After Hubel and Wiesel, l977.)
 
 
 
 

Back to tutorial 5 notes


Hormones

Hormones can affect the growth and development of neurons

Metamorphosis (§4.4.1)

Tadpole to frog: stimulated by thyroxine (thyroid hormone) inhibited by prolactin.
Differentiation of neurons supplying sensory hairs in caterpillar (larva) to the 'gin trap' in moth chrysalis (pupa)is promoted by ecdysteroid and opposed by juvenile hormone.

Sex differences (§4.4.2)

Hormones such as testosterone and oestrogen can have organising (early) and activating (late) effects, but the effects differ between species.

In rats and monkeys, testosterone has a maculinising action by preventing feminisation. (Fetal mammals become female unless exposed to testosterone.) Without this organising effect, normal male behaviour cannot be activated at a later stage by testosterone.
Organising effects can cause neural changes. In rat CNS, testosterone increases the size of the medial preoptic nucleus and amygdala and also the spinal nucleus bulbocavernosus

Different effects are found in birds. In Japanese quail, testosterone and oestrogen are both feminising. But in zebra finches, testosterone and oestrogen are masculising and promote singing by promoting growth of the higher vocal centre and robustus archistriatalis. (Here, oestrogen is the organiser, and testosterone the activator.)
These different effects are possible, because testosterone can be converted to oestrogen inside cells. Unlike peptide hormones, which act on outer cell membrane receptors, steroid hormones act on receptors inside the cells.


Human development (§4.5) is also subject to similar influences of genes, external environment and hormones, although the effects can only be observed, and not tested by experiment (why not?)
 

Return to Tutorial FIVE notes

or

 Back to BBB Home